Happy birthday? How your birthdate contributes to success or failure due to the “relative age effect”

Have you ever stopped to wonder the difference a day can make? Say, being born December 31st or January 1st? This small 24-hour window could be the difference between competing in elite vs. recreational sport, or being identified as “gifted”, or (mis)diagnosed with a learning disability in school. Many youth development systems (e.g., sport and education) rely on arbitrary cut-off dates to group individuals into cohorts, which result in age differences among children within a single cohort. Consider that an 11-month age difference among 10-year-old children represents 10% of their total life experience. While this difference may seem trivial, it can lead to a phenomenon known as the relative age effect (RAE). The RAE tends to advantage the oldest individuals in a cohort and disadvantage their relatively younger peers. The implications of RAEs are broad, extending from sport to education to health and wellbeing. For example, the relatively youngest students have been found to achieve lower grades and have poorer school attendance rates (Cobley et al., 2009a), are less likely to attend or complete post-secondary school (Bedard & Dhuey, 2006; Dhuey et al., 2017), demonstrate lower levels of self-esteem (Thompson et al., 2004), and have higher rates of suicide (Thompson et al., 1999) and incarceration for juvenile crimes (Dhuey et al., 2017).

Why does this happen? The answers, it turns out, are fairly straight-forward. Relative age effects tend to occur when individuals are selected based upon their perceived ability and then placed into differing steams (e.g., gifted programs, competitive sport) that offer dissimilar opportunities and resources. The problem is that coaches, teachers, and parents often confuse “talent” with “age.” While they assume they are selecting the most talented children for these elite opportunities, often it is just the oldest among the cohort.

Within sport, we tend to see RAEs most frequently in culturally valued activities such as hockey in Canada, or soccer in England. Unfortunately, RAEs can lead to younger athletes dropping out (e.g., Lemez et al., 2014) thereby missing out on the many positive outcomes associated with sport participation.

Ice hockey has been one of the most frequently studied sports with RAEs occurring as early as age 7 (Hancock et al., 2013), peaking at elite junior levels (≈ 16-21 years), then leveling off somewhat in the NHL (Cobley et al., 2009b). However, there is evidence suggesting that younger athletes who “survive” the system may ultimately become better as a result (Gibbs et al., 2011).

Within sport, RAE is a popular topic among academics and policy makers, having been discussed at previous Sport Canada Research Initiative conferences and featured in the SIRCuit. The RAE has also captured the attention of the popular press, having been profiled in such best-selling books such as Gladwell’s (2008) Outliers: The Story of Success and Levitt and Dubner’s (2009) SuperFreakonomics, sports magazines like Sports Illustrated (Levy, 2011), and on television programs such as 60 Minutes (CBS Interactive, 2012).

The RAE problem that plagues education and sport can lead to inequitable experiences and opportunities for individuals. Researchers must work in collaboration with sport and education stakeholders to develop feasible and attractive solutions to the RAE so that all individuals, regardless of their birthdates, have equal opportunities to experience success in academics and athletics.

Subscribe to updates

News travels fast. Delivered straight to your inbox, SIRC’s daily newsletter will ensure you stay connected with the latest news, events, jobs, and knowledge in Canadian sport.

latest articles

SIGN up for Canadian sport daily

News travels fast. Delivered straight to your inbox, SIRC’s daily newsletter will ensure you stay connected with the latest news, events, jobs, and knowledge in Canadian sport.

Skip to content